Amid the mist and suds, a good shower can relax not only your body but also your mind, unleashing streams of consciousness, clarity and creativity.

Research shows that what is known as the “shower effect” also can occur outside the shower, and many of our best thoughts don’t happen at work or school — but occur while going about our days with ideas incubating in the background.

In a 2019 study, 98 professional writers and 87 physicists recorded their most creative idea each day, as well as what they were doing and thinking when it struck them.

While most of the ideas occurred at work, 20% of their most meaningful ideas came while doing something else — washing dishes or taking a shower. Notably, the ideas the writers and physicists had away from their jobs were self-assessed to be just as creative and important as the ones they had at work.

This is in itself pretty impressive, said Jonathan Schooler, professor of psychological and brain sciences at the University of California at Santa Barbara and author of the study.

“How many things can you do as successfully in the shower as you can while you’re at your desk?” he said.

But those spontaneous thoughts in the shower had another advantage: They were more likely to be “aha” experiences — modern Archimedes-in-the-bathtub “Eureka!” moments — that overcame a creative impasse.

The shower effect on creativity is the product of mind-wandering, where your thoughts are anywhere but on the task at hand.

Historically, researchers focused on the negative effects of mind-wandering, which can disrupt performance and has been associated with unhappiness.

But the nature of the mind-wandering matters; Schooler’s research has found that people whose mind wandered about something they’re interested in were happier than when they were on task.

“So, whereas mind-wandering can be problematic, what I’ve come to call ‘mind-wondering,’ which is sort of playful, curious mind-wandering, may actually be helpful,” Schooler said.

Its benefits seem to be relatively widespread. One study of more than 1,100 respondents reported that their moments of insight came during mind-wandering in the shower (30%), in transit (13%) or during exercise (11%).

There is a “sweet spot” to how engaging the activity is and its impact on creative thought: too little, and it is boring; too much, and it leaves little attention for ideating.

A recent study reported that having free-flowing, spontaneous thoughts during a moderately engaging activity, but not a boring one, boosted creativity.

Over 300 university students were first asked to come up with as many novel uses as possible for ordinary objects, such as a brick or a paper clip, in 90 seconds. This is known as an alternative-uses task, which tests creativity.

The students then watched either a boring video of two men hanging laundry or a more engaging clip of the infamous deli scene from “When Harry Met Sally.” These activities gave time for the students’ ideas to incubate and for their minds to wander.

When the students were tested again, their mind-wandering led to more creative ideas, but only for the students who watched the moderately engaging deli scene.

An activity such as taking a shower or a walk (or watching a rom-com video) “takes some of your attention so that they can influence your thoughts, but doesn’t take all of it so that you have no attention for these creative ideas that are incubating in the background,” said Zachary Irving, assistant professor of philosophy of cognitive science at the University of Virginia and author of the study.

Our ability to generate novel ideas and creative thoughts probably arises from our brain’s “default mode network” (DMN), a constellation of brain regions that are active when our thoughts are turned inward, such as when mind-wandering. The evidence for the DMN’s role in creativity had been correlational, but a January 2022 study reported the first direct evidence that the DMN is causally linked to creative thinking.

In the research, 13 patients were undergoing brain surgery to have their tumors removed. They had to be awake so that the surgeons could map out the cortex with electrical stimulation, which inhibits a very specific area of the brain for a short period of time, said Ben Shofty, functional neurosurgeon and assistant professor at the University of Utah.

The patients underwent the alternate-uses task, listing uncommon uses for common items. When the surgeons stimulated a brain area inside the DMN, the patient’s ability to think of creative ideas temporarily ceased.

At baseline, one patient came up with many creative uses for a tire — a cushion, a garden fountain or cut into pieces for jewelry. But when the researchers electrically inhibited parts of her DMN, she struggled to think of new ways to use a pencil — “you can draw with it, you can draw cartoons, hmm, mostly draw.”

Creativity is “a basic part of what makes us human. We use it all the time,” said Shofty, the neurosurgeon. “So, I think understanding that mechanism and how it works is super important for us to understand ourselves and what makes us human.”

Richard Sima is a neuroscientist-turned-science journalist.

© 2023 Washington Post